The current study examined how being a Holocaust Surviving Offspring (HSO) and his or her attachment orientation might affect mid-life offspring’s reactions to parental loss. Sample included 146 Israel mid-life adults: 61 who were grieving for their aging parents and 85 non-grieving participants. Each group included HSO and non-HSO who completed attachment orientations and mental health questionnaires. Findings indicated that grieving participants reported worse mental health than non-grieving participants only if their recently dead parent was a Holocaust survivor and they scored relatively high on attachment anxiety. The discussion deal with the mechanisms underlying adjustment to parental death during mid-life.
Losing a parent is a stressful experience (see
To date, attachment orientations are conceptualized in terms of a two-dimensional space defined by two roughly orthogonal dimensions, attachment anxiety and attachment-related avoidance (e.g.,
There is evidence that attachment orientations are relevant for explaining individual differences in grief reactions. Secure attachment allows a person to work through the loss experience and return to normal functioning (
Adult attachment studies found no association between avoidant attachment and heightened distress following the death of a relationship partner (
Although all the studies conducted on the attachment-grief link have focused on the death of a spouse, we hypothesize that attachment anxiety would also heighten distress of mid-life adults coping with the death of a parent. This is a novel hypothesis as no published study to date has examined the role of attachment insecurities in the context of parental death during mid-life.
As mentioned earlier, a person’s attachment orientations seemed to depend on parents’ sensitivity and responsiveness to his or her needs for protection and support in times of need. Research has shown that parents’ trauma-related history can lead to major difficulties in properly fulfilling these parental duties (
Much have been also written about Holocaust survivors’ complicated relationship with their offspring (
Now days, more than seventy years after the end of WWII, most of Holocaust survivors who are still alive are in their 80s-90s under the care of their offspring. Caring for an aging parent may be particularly demanding and challenging for HSO (
A still more challenging event for HSO is the death of their old-age survivor parent. Although caring for an aging parent might be quite arduous, HSO can still maintain a vital relationship with the traumatized parent. In contrast, HSO who have lost their parent, although being “liberated” from the daily hassles and burden involved in caring for an aging parent, are confronted with the “final separation” from the traumatized parent and their irrevocable failure to protect him or her from death. Moreover, parental death can be particularly distressing for HSO, who care for their parents with great devotion, partly due to their ongoing need to compensate for parents’ past losses and partly due to their guilt feelings (
Interviews with Holocaust survivors also revealed that while expecting their children to enjoy life, they also passed on a sense of obligation to the dead, which might pose further difficulties to HSO’s grief (
In order to examine the above hypotheses, we conducted a cross-sectional study with Israeli mid-life adults who lost one of their parent in the last three months before the study (grieving group) or did not lose a parent during this period (non-grieving group ( . In each of these two groups, we sampled participants who were either HSO or non-HSO. All the participants completed scales tapping their attachment orientations and their current psychological well-being and psychological distress. Our prediction was that differences between grieving and non-grieving groups in well-being and distress would be moderated by attachment anxiety and lost parent’s Holocaust background. Specifically, grieving participants will report higher distress and lower well-being than non-grieving controls mainly when attachment anxiety scores would be relatively high and the lost parent was a Holocaust survivor.
Beyond examining these predictions, we will explore the role of attachment avoidance in explaining emotional responses to parental death. We could not provide ad hoc predictions due to the inconclusive findings concerning the contribution of avoidant attachment to the grief process. We will also explore possible differences between HSO and non-HSO in their level of attachment insecurities.
The sample included 146 Israeli adults (52 men and 94 women), ranging in age from 40 to 71 years (M = 55.2, SD = 7.81), who volunteered to participate in the study without receiving any monetary compensation. This specific age range was chosen due to two main reasons. First, many people in this age range are dealing with the loss of their parents (
We sampled two groups of participants: (a) participants who were grieving for one of their parents who died in the last three months before the study (grieving group, n = 61) and (b) participants who did not lose a parent in the last three months (non-grieving group, n = 85). In the grieving group, the other parent was alive at the time of the study or died over a year before. In the non-grieving group, both parents were alive at the time of the study or one parent died over a year before. In each of these groups, we attempted to equally represent HSO and non-HSO participants. Holocaust survivor offspring (n = 32 in the grieving group and n = 40 in the non-grieving group) were defined as participants whose at least one parent (the dead parent among grievers) was at concentration camps, ghettos, or was partisan during WWII, thereby reducing the heterogeneity of the Holocaust experiences. Non-HSO (n = 29 in the grieving group and n = 45 in the non-grieving group) were defined as participants whose parents immigrated to Israel from Europe before 1939 and their immediate family had no Holocaust background.
There was a significant difference in age between the two groups, t (1, 14 4 ) = -3.22, p = .002. Participants in the grieving group were older (M = 57.57, SD = 6.41) than those in the non-grieving group (M = 53.48, SD = 8.30). No significant age difference was found between HSO and non-HSO, t (1, 144) = -.82, p = .41. In order to control for this unexpected group difference, we introduced participants’ age as a covariate in the analyses examining the study’s predictions. No other significant differences were found between study groups in gender, marital status, education, religiosity, and income.
In the grieving group, 50% of the participants reported that their father died in the last three months before the study and 50% reported that their mother died during this period. In 36.1% of grieving participants, the other parent was alive at the time of the study. In the non-grieving groups, 45.9 % of the participants reported that both parents were alive at the time of the study.
Twenty-five percent of HSO reported that only the father was a Holocaust survivor, 18.05% reported that only their mother was a survivor, and 56.95% reported that both parents survived the Holocaust. 50% of them were at concentration camps, 43% were at ghettos, and 7% were partisans during WWII. No significant differences in these variables were found between the grieving HSO and non-grieving HSO groups, χ2s < 6.01, p > .06.
After receiving ethic approval from the university, participants were recruited using direct person-to-person solicitation in social clubs and community centers. In addition, we posted on-line advertisements and called for participation through social networks. We sought first for HSO participants and conducted a phone interview about their parents’ Holocaust background, whether their parents are alive, and whether one parent deceased during the last three months. We then asked participants who filled the inclusion criteria their permission to participate in the study. After collecting data in the HSO groups, we sought for controls who matched HSO in age and gender and conducted a similar phone interview to check whether they filled the inclusion criteria. From these phone interviews, 182 participants fitted the inclusion criteria, but only 146 agreed to participate in the study (rejection rate of 19.79%).
After fitting to the inclusion criteria, participants were told that the study deals with quality of life and that it would demand from them to complete a 20-min survey. Participants who agreed to take part in the study signed an informed consent form and completed a battery of three randomly ordered self-report scales tapping attachment orientations and mental health. All the questionnaires were written in Hebrew.
Participants’ attachment orientations were assessed with the 36-item Experiences in Close Relationships scale (ECR;
The correlation between the two attachment scores was positive and significant, r = .50, p < .001. Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVAs) for grieving for the death of a parent (yes, no) and HSO (HSO, non-HSO) revealed no significant main effects or interactions on attachment anxiety and avoidance, all Fs < . 1.03 , all ps > . 382 . That is, study groups did not differ in dispositional attachment orientations.
Mental health was assessed with a brief 15-item version of the Mental Health Inventory (MHI;
The study’s predictions were examined with hierarchical regression analyses for psychological well-being and psychological distress with parental death (yes, no), HSO (HSO, non-HSO), and attachment orientations (anxiety, avoidance) as the predictors. Due to the sample size and limitations in statistical power, we could not enter the two attachment scores and all their interactions in a single regression analysis. Therefore, we conducted separated regressions for each attachment score. That is, we computed a set of regressions with parental death, HSO and attachment anxiety as the predictors and another set of regressions with parental death, HSO, and attachment avoidance as the predictors. However, since the two attachment scores were strongly associated in the current sample (r = .50), we need to examine the contribution of each attachment score (anxiety or avoidance) while controlling for its association with the other attachment score. For this purpose, before conducting the main hierarchical regression analyses, we regressed each attachment score on the other attachment score (anxiety as predicted by avoidance, avoidance as predicted by anxiety) and computed for each participant the residual score of the regression – i.e., a participant’s specific attachment score that was unexplained by the other attachment score. Then, we entered these residual scores in the main hierarchical regressions together with parental death and HSO as the predictors.
The main hierarchical regressions included three steps. In first step, we entered parental death (an effect-coded variable comparing grieving participants, +1, to non-grieving ones, -1), being a HSO (an effect-coded variable compering HSO, +1, to non- HSO, -1), and the residual score of an attachment dimension (either attachment anxiety or avoidance) in order to examine their unique main effects. In the second step, we entered all the two-way interactions between parental death, being a HSO, and the residual score of an attachment dimension (three interactions). In the third step, we entered the three-way interaction between the predictors. In all these regressions, we included participants’ age as a covariate. Simple Slope Effects tests were conducted for examining the source of the significant two- and three-way interactions.
In this section, we report findings from two hierarchical regressions examining main effects and interactions of parental death, being a HSO, and attachment anxiety (unexplained by attachment avoidance) on psychological well-being and psychological distress while controlling for age as a covariate (see relevant statistics in
In testing the study’ prediction, Simple Slope Effects tests revealed that grieving participants reported lower well-being than non-grievers only if they were HSO and scored relatively high on attachment anxiety (+1 SD), b = -.36, SE = .13, t = -2.75, p = .006. However, among non-HSO who scored relatively high on attachment anxiety (+1 SD), grieving participants reported higher well-being than non-grievers, b = .34, SE = .14, t = 2.48, p = .014. Among HSO or non-HSO who scored relatively low on attachment anxiety, grieving participants did not significantly differ from non-grievers in well-being, bs < -.24, all ps > .070. As can be seen in Figure 1, the lowest level of well-being was reported by HSO who were grieving a parent’s death and scored high on attachment anxiety. But unexpectedly, non-HSO who scored high on attachment anxiety seemed to benefit (heightened well-being) from the death of a parent.
The regression model for psychological distress was also statistically significant and explained 37.60% of the variance (see Table 1). In Step 1, the main effects for parental death and attachment anxiety were significant. Fitting the study’s prediction, participants who were grieving for a parent’s death reported higher levels of distress than non-grievers. In addition, participants scoring higher on attachment anxiety also reported higher levels of distress (see Table 1). In Step 2, we found a significant interaction for HSO x anxiety, which was qualified by a significant three-way interaction in Step 3 (see
Simple Slope Effects tests revealed that that grieving participants reported higher distress than non-grievers only if they were HSO and scored relatively high on attachment anxiety (+1 SD), b = .25, SE = .11, t = 2.33, p = .021. Among non-HSO who scored relatively high on attachment anxiety (+1 SD) or among HSO or non-HSO who scored relatively low on attachment anxiety, grieving participants did not significantly differ from non-grievers in distress, bs < .14, all ps > .208. As can be seen in Figure 2, the highest level of distress was reported by HSO who were grieving a parent’s death and scored high on attachment anxiety.
In this section, we reported findings from two hierarchical regressions examining main effects and interactions of parental death, being a HSO, and attachment avoidance (unexplained by attachment anxiety) on well-being and distress. The regression model for psychological distress was not significant and explained only 10.53% of the variance, F(8, 135) = 1.99. However, the regression model for psychological well-being was significant and explained 13.60% of the variance, F(8, 135) = 2.66, p = .006. Beyond the significant main effect of parental death and the interaction between HSO and parental death that were reported above, the unique and interactive effects of avoidance were not significant bs < -.11, all ps > .147.
Regression Analyses for Psychological Well-Being and Psychological Distress as a Function of Parental Death, HSO, and Attachment Anxiety Note. PD = Parental Death; HSO = Holocaust Survivor Offspring; *** p < .001
Effect
b (SE)
t
p
β
__95% CI_ LL UL
Psychological well-being
Step 1
Parental death
-.180 (.072)
-2.49
.013
-.200
-.322 -.037
HSO
-.042 (.070)
-0.63
.530
-.040
-.177 .091
Attachment anxiety
-.380 (.080)
-4.82
< .001
-.368
-.535 -.223
R2 Step 1
.195
Step 2
HSO x PD
-.200 (.067)
-2.98
.003
-.223
-.332 -.067
HSO × anxiety
-.197 (.077)
-2.45
.012
-.191
-.350 -.043
PD × anxiety
.105 (.077)
1.36
.175
.102
-.047 .258
∆R2 Step 2
.083
Step 3
HSO × PD × anxiety
-.156 (.076)
-2.04
.043
-.150
-.307 -.004
∆R2 Step 3
.022
Total R2
.300
F (8, 135)
7.24***
Psychological distress
Step 1
Parental death
.120 (.057)
2.07
.040
.164
.005 .234
HSO
.060 (.054)
1.09
.278
.082
-.048 .167
Attachment anxiety
.304 (.063)
4.81
< .001
.365
.179 .429
R2 Step 1
.207
Step 2
HSO x PD
.053 (.055)
0.96
.340
.073
-.056 .162
HSO × anxiety
.161 (.064)
2.52
.013
.193
.034 .288
PD × anxiety
.010 (.063)
0.17
.868
.012
-.115 .136
∆R2 Step 2
.041
Step 3
HSO × PD × anxiety
.145 (.063)
2.29
.023
.171
.019 .269
∆R2 Step 3
.027
Total R2
.276
F (8, 135)
6.43***
Psychological Well-Being as a Function of Parental Death, HSO, and Attachment Anxiety
Psychological Distress as a Function of Parental Death, HSO, and Attachment Anxiety
Our initial analyses revealed significant differences in mental health between mid-life adults who were dealing with parental death and those whose parents were still alive. These findings expand the literature about the difficulties that arise among grieving offspring, since it usually focuses on parental loss during childhood (e.g.,
In addition, our study suggests that parental trauma history and offspring’s attachment orientations matter. We found that losing a parent was associated with higher levels of distress and lower levels of well-being only when grievers were HSO and reported relatively high attachment anxiety. These findings are in line with previous findings showing that HSO tends to exhibit heightened emotional problems only after exposure to life stressors (e.g.,
From a more salutogenic perspective, our findings indicated that possession of psychological resources for coping with parental death, such attachment security (relatively low scores in attachment anxiety) can prevent emotional and relational problems among grievers. This conclusion is in line with
Our findings were also in line with the existing literature regarding the contribution of attachment anxiety to emotional problems and maladjustment following death of a loved one (see
Another intriguing, but unexpected, set of findings revealed that parental death was associated with higher well-being among non-HSO who scored high on attachment anxiety. When caring for an aging parent, attachment-anxious people might be over-involved and experience heightened levels of emotional burden (
With regard to attachment-related avoidance, findings indicated no significant contribution to variations in well-being and distress following parental death. This finding is consistent with previous findings showing that avoidant attachment does not contribute to heightened distress following the death of a loved one (
Our study has several limitations. The main limitation was the difficulty to recruit participants to take part in a study dealing with parental death right after the loss, when the hardship and the pain are still so fresh. In addition, since the population of Holocaust survivors is decreasing dramatically each passing day, it was even more challenging to recruit HSO for both non-grieving and grieving groups. These difficulties have resulted in a relatively small sample, which restricted the study’s statistical power and the generalizability of findings. Additionally, our findings were based on self-report measures, which can be biased by social desirability, and a cross-sectional design that prevented us from making strong inferences about direction of causality. Further studies should include behavioral observations or less explicit measures of attachment, well-being and distress and rely on prospective longitudinal designs that can follow the contribution of parental death and attachment orientations to mental health over time.
In the current study we focused on HSO’s attachment orientation as a proxy of the relationship satisfaction they had with the traumatized parent. Future studies can benefit from examining other variables that can highlight the quality of such a relationship, such as parent’s attachment orientations or specific aspects of the relationship (e.g., intimacy, self-disclosure, investment). Also, future studies using larger samples can consider if both parents are dead or one of them is still alive. In addition, since elderly parental death has hardly been investigated until now, it is important for future research to assess how this event is perceived by their grieving children. This assessment is important due to previous findings showing an association between how much the event is evaluated as traumatic and the emergence of emotional problems mainly among attachment-avoidant people (e.g.,
The current study has several theoretical and clinical implications. Theoretically, it reinforces the understanding of grief reaction after parental death among mid-life adults. The addition of dead parent’s background and offspring’s attachment orientations takes this knowledge one step further by highlighting how factors that related to parent-child relationship contribute to the emergence of emotional and relational problems following this loss.
Clinically, the current findings reinforce the importance of developing specific intervention programs for mid-life people who are dealing with elderly parental death. As mentioned earlier, little is known about the difficulties this population is dealing with. It is important to consider their needs for special adjustments when they turn to treatment. However, in most cases therapists and therapeutic centers are not qualified or knowledgeable about those necessary accommodations (