6723 PSOCIAL 2422-619X Universidad de Buenos Aires Argentina hugosimkin@sociales.uba.ar 672373384009 Sin sección On the expression of negation in Mandarin Chinese and Spanish https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5175-0379 Li Feifei feifeili@uestc.edu.cn University of Electronic Science and Technology of China University of Electronic Science and Technology of China China Diciembre 2022 8 2 03 09 2022 25 10 2022 Abstract

A macroparametric division between Double Negation (DN) and Negative Concord (NC) languages has been postulated in the literature on negation. Mandarin Chinese and Spanish have been classified as a DN and a NC language respectively. However, the interpretation of the expression of negation in NC versus DN languages is not clear-cut. This study aims to introduce and compare the interpretation of multiple negative expressions, fragment answers to negative questions and two experimental studies in these two languages. The results from the comparison show that unexpected interpretations of double negation and single negation are possible in both Mandarin Chinese and Spanish respectively under certain prosodic and gestural conditions. Accordingly, it allows us to question the parametric division between DN and NC languages.

Resumen

En la literatura sobre la negación se ha postulado una división macroparamétrica entre las lenguas de doble negación (DN) y de concordancia negativa (NC). El chino mandarín y el español han sido clasificados como lenguas de DN y NC respectivamente. Sin embargo, la interpretación de la expresión de la negación en las lenguas de NC frente a las de DN no está clara. Este estudio propone introducir y comparar la interpretación de las expresiones de negación múltiple, las respuestas fragmentarias a preguntas negativas y dos estudios experimentales en estas dos lenguas. Los resultados de la comparación muestran que son posibles interpretaciones inesperadas de la doble negación y de la negación simple tanto en chino mandarín como en español, respectivamente, bajo ciertas condiciones prosódicas y gestuales. En consecuencia, nos permite cuestionar la división paramétrica entre las lenguas DN y NC

摘要

传统类型学根据否定表达的不同将语言分为双重否定语言和否定一致语言。汉语普通话和西班牙语分别被归类为双重否定语言和否定一致语言。然而,双重否定语言和否定一致语言中关于否定的释义并不清晰。本研究旨在介绍和比较汉语普通话和西班牙语中多个否定表达的释义、否定问题片段回答的释义以及两个实验研究。比较结果表明,在一定的韵律和肢体语言条件下,汉语普通话和西班牙中分别出现了单重否定和双重否定的释义。因此,关于双重否定和否定一致语言的参数划分需要进一步研究。

Keywords negation Mandarin Chinese Spanish Palabras clave negación chino mandarín español 關鍵詞 关键词 否定 汉语普通话
INTRODUCTION

Negation has been classified in the literature into two different typologies (i.e., Double Negation and Negative Concord) when multiple negative expressions co-occur within the boundaries of a single clause (Biberauer & Zeijlstra, 2012; Giannakidou, 1998, 2006; Van der Wouden, 1994; Zeijlstra, 2004; and others).

In propositional logic, double negation (henceforth DN) is related to double negation elimination (Law of Double Negation, Horn 1989), by which if --p is true, then p is true (i.e., --p ⟹ p), and conversely, if p is true, then it is not the case that the statement is not true (i.e., p⟹--p). In so-called double negation languages, such as standard English, modern Dutch and modern German, the Law of Double Negation is expected to apply, in such a way that, when multiple negative expressions co-occur within the boundaries of a simple clause, a positive reading is inferred. This is due to the fact that one of these elements cancels out the meaning of the second one, yielding an affirmative interpretation. According to the examples below:

(1) a. Mary will not not show up (= Mary will show up.) (Standard English)

(example from Zeijlstra 2004:58, ex. (65))

b. Nobody will not be touched by this movie (= Everybody will be touched by this

movie.).

(example from Zeijlstra 2004:59, ex. (67a))

In so-called negative concord (henceforth NC) languages there are multiple ways through which the speaker can achieve negation. Using the marker of sentential negation is one way. Another is through the use of certain indefinite expressions, termed n-words (Laka 1990), such as French rien, personne, and Spanish nadie, nada, etc. The properties of n-words are different from those of the negative quantifiers in DN languages. N-words are indefinite expressions that need to be used together with other negative operators within a sentential domain in order to express negation in a sentence in NC languages. According to the examples below:

(2) No hay nada.

not have nothing

‘There is nothing.’

In the traditional literature, Mandarin Chinese (henceforth MC) has been characterized as a DN language; Spanish has been classified as a NC language. However, certain conditions have been found to lead to a shift from a single negation (henceforth SN) reading to a DN reading in NC languages and a DN reading to a SN reading in DN languages (Blanchette, 2017; Blanchette et al., 2018; Espinal & Prieto, 2011; Espinal et al., 2016; Huddlestone, 2010; Puskás, 2012; Thornton et al., 2016; Tubau, 2008; Zeijlstra, 2010).

This study investigates the expression and interpretation of negation in Mandarin Chinese and Spanish. It aims to achieve a better understanding of the interpretation of multiple negative expressions and fragment answers to negative questions in these two languages. Moreover, it aims to compare two experimental studies in MC and Spanish to determine whether these two different typological languages have a robust empirical basis in light of different linguistic and non-linguistic conditions in common. Consequently, this research questions the parametric division between DN and NC languages.

Negative expressions in Mandarin Chinese vs. Spanish

In MC, negation is expressed by meaning of several negative expressions. Sentential negation is usually expressed by means of the negative sentential marker 没有méi(yǒu) ‘not’ and 不 ‘not’ ( Li & Thompson, 1981; Lü, 1999), as illustrated below:

(3) a. Tā méi(yǒu) qù gōngyuán.

(s)he not.have go park

‘(S)he didn’t go to the park.’

b. Tā qù gōngyuán.

(s)he not go park

‘(S)he don’t/won’t go to the park.’

and 没(有)méi(yǒu) differ in several aspects[1]. The basic difference between 不 and 没(有)méi(yǒu) is that 不 is not limited by the time frame (Li & Thompson 1981). Thus, 不 can be used in the present, future or past, while 没(有)méi(yǒu) is most commonly used for negating in the past. In (3a), 没(有)méi(yǒu) is used in the past, while in (3b), 不 is used in the sentence in the present/future. Another difference is that 不 is used to negate a judgement or a subjective will (Liu et.al, 2010; Lü, 1999), while 没(有)méi(yǒu) is used to negate the occurrence of an action. In (3a), 没(有)méi(yǒu) negates the occurrence of the action of going to the park; in (3b), 不 negates the speaker’s subjective will, for the speaker does not want to go to the park.

Beyond the sentential negative marker 没(有)méi(yǒu) and 不bù, in MC negation can also be expressed by means of other negative expressions such as 没(有)人méi(yǒu)rén ‘no one’ and 没(有)什么méi(yǒu)shénme ‘nothing’ (nominal negation), as illustrated below:

(4) Méirén rènshi tā.

nobody know 3sg

‘Nobody knows him/her.’

(example from Li 2007:302, ex. (28))

(5) Méiyǒushénme shì yǒnghéng de.

not.have.thing/not.have.what is everlasting PART

‘Nothing is everlasting.’

In addition to the most important negative makers 没(有)méi(yǒu) and 不 ‘not’, and the argumental negative expressions, MC has several other negative expressions, such as 别bié ‘don’t’, 甭béng ‘don’t, needn’t’, 无 ‘not’; 非fēi ‘non-; not’; and 未wèi ‘not yet’, which have some characteristic use (Ernst, 1995; Li & Thompson, 1981; Lü, 1999; Xiao & McEnery, 2008; Zhuang & Liu, 2011; Zhu, 2011). 别Bié ‘don’t’ and béng ‘don’t, needn’t’ are negative adverbs typically used in imperatives. 无 ‘not’ is always used in written language. 非Fēi ‘non-; not’ is usually used as a negative prefix. 未 Wèi ‘not yet’ is usually used in compound words, such as 未曾wèicéng ‘not ever’. See the examples below.

(6) Bié dòng

not move

‘Don’t move.’

(7) Béng zháojí.

not worry

‘Don’t worry.’

(8) Tā de yǎnshén wú sīháo wèijù.

she PART eye not a bit fear

‘Her eyes showed no fear.’

(9) fēi huìyuán

not member

‘non-member’

(10) Wǒ wèicéng lái guò zhèlǐ.

I not.ever come PART here

‘I never come here.’

In Spanish, no ‘no’, ni ‘not’, nada ‘nothing’, nadie ‘nobody’, nunca ‘never’, ninguno ‘none’ etc. can be used to express negation[2]. The most important negative sentential marker in Spanish is no ‘no’, which is usually used prior to verbs. Ni ‘not’ is an adverb and is always used with no ‘no’ or in the construction of ni….ni. Nada ‘nothing’ and nadie ‘nobody’ are negative indefinites. Nunca ‘never’ is an adverb. Ninguno ‘none’ can be used as either an adjective or a pronoun. See the following examples:

(11) No canta más.

no sing.3.sg more

‘She/He does not sing anymore.’

(12) Ni Paco ni Pepe estudian.

no Paco not Pepe study

‘Neither Paco nor Pepe study.’

(13) No hay nada.

no have nothing

‘There is nothing.’

(14) No hay nadie en la habitación.

no have nobody in the room

‘There is nobody in the room.’

(15) Nunca lo haré.

Never it do.1st.pl.

‘I will never do it.’

(16) No he leído ninguno de estos libros.

no have.1 read none of these book

‘I have not read any of these books.’

Multiple negative expressions in Mandarin Chinese vs. Spanish

As we have already mentioned, languages are categorized into those that use double negation (DN) and those that use negative concord (NC) in the linguistics literature (Biberauer & Zeijlstra, 2012; Giannakidou, 1998, 2006; Van der Wouden, 1994; Zeijlstra, 2004).

In so-called DN languages, when multiple negative expressions combine within a single clause, the expected interpretation of the sentence is an affirmative reading, as illustrated by the English sentence She didn’t eat nothing, interpreted (in standard usage) to mean “She ate something”.

On the other hand, in so-called NC languages, the expected interpretation of a sentence with multiple negative expressions is a SN reading, as exemplified in Spanish by Ella no comió nada, which is interpreted to mean “She ate nothing”.

MC has been characterized as a DN language in the literature (Cheng & Li, 1991; Ding, et al. 1999; Lü, 1985). Studies on negation in MC highlight that, in this language, when two negative expressions occur within the boundaries of a single clause, a positive meaning is conveyed (Cheng & Li, 1991; Ding et al., 1999:20; Huang & Liao, 2007; Lü, 1985:247, 1990:243; Zhang, 2012) as a result of applying the law of double negation. That is, each negative expression morphosyntactically corresponds to one negative operator (¬) in the semantic representation. Consequently, when two negative expressions occur in one sentence, the semantics of the sentence includes two negations and they cancel each other out, therefore making the sentence positive (¬¬p ⟹ p). Thus, it is important to keep in mind that combinations of two negative expressions, such as 没(有)méi(yŏu) ‘not’ and 不 ‘not’, lead to DN reading (Ding et al., 1999:201; Lü, 1985:247; Yang, 2011: 209; Zhou et al., 2014:337; Zhuang, 2015:127), as illustrated in the following examples ( from Zhou, 2014:337, exs. (10) and (12); Yang, 2011:209, exs. (6)):

(17) a. Tā huì lái.

3sg not will not come

‘(S)he won’t not come.’ (= ‘(S)he will come.’)

b. Wǒ méi(yǒu) xǐhuān xiàndài yīnyuè.

I not.have not like modern music

‘It is/was not the case that I don’t/didn’t like modern music.’ (= ‘I like(d) modern music.’)

c. Tā .huì méi(yǒu) dài qián lái.

3sg not.will not.have carry money come

‘It is not the case that (s)he will not bring money with him/her.’ (= ‘(S)he will bring money with him/her.’)

d. Wǒ méi(yǒu) méi(yǒu) qù guò Měiguó.

I not.have not.have go PART America

‘I didn’t not go to America.’ (= ‘I went to America.’)

Spanish has been characterized as a NC language in the literature (Bosque, 1980; Sánchez, 1999). In Spanish, multiple morphosyntactically negative expressions yield the composition of a single semantic negation. When it combines multiple negative expressions it typically leads to a single negation reading (¬p) in spite of the fact that within the limit of a sentence the negative marker combines with an indefinite expression that can also convey a negative meaning. In Spanish, the combination of multiple negative expressions (negative maker no, and n-words such as nadie ‘nobody’, nada ‘nothing’ and nunca ‘never’) generally yields a SN reading. Consider the examples below:

(18) a. No vino nadie.

not come nobody

‘Nobody came.’

(example from Bosque 1980: 29, ex (1); glosses are mine)

b. Juan no dice nunca la verdad.

Juan not say never the truth

‘Juan never tells the truth.’

(example from Sánchez 1999: 2569, ex (16b), glosses are mine)

c. No hay nada.

not have nothing

‘There is nothing.’

As illustrated above, in example 18(a) the combination of negative expressions illustrates the negative marker no ‘not’ plus the n-word nadie ‘nobody’; in 18(b) it shows the negative element no ‘not’ plus the n-word nunca ‘never’; in 18(c) the negative element no ‘not’ plus the n-word nada ‘nothing’. Even though there are two negative expressions in each sentence, they yield the composition of a single semantic negation. In short, such types of structures with multiple negative expressions, viz., a negative marker no plus a n-word, yield a SN reading in Spanish.

Fragment negative answers in Mandarin Chinese vs. Spanish

In the traditional literature, fragment negative quantifiers are claimed to convey a Double Negation (DN) reading when used as fragment answers – e.g., 没(有)人méi(yǒu)rén ‘no one’ – to negative questions in the so-called Double Negation languages (Cheng and Li, 1991; Ding et al., 1999; Lü 1985). In contrast, negative indefinite expressions are claimed to convey a single negation (SN) reading when used as fragment answers to negative questions in the so-called Negative Concord (NC) languages. However, the differences in the interpretation of fragment answers in NC versus DN languages is not clear-cut. The SN interpretation of fragment n-words as answers to negative questions has been demonstrated in DN languages such as Standard English (Blanchette, 2017; Blanchette & Nadeu, 2018). A DN interpretation for n-words when used as fragment answers to negative questions has also been shown under certain prosodic and gestural conditions in NC languages such as Catalan, Spanish (Espinal & Prieto, 2011; Espinal et. al, 2016; Prieto et. al, 2013) and French (Depréz & Yeaton, 2018).

In Mandarin Chinese, when negative fragments such as 没(有)人méi(yǒu)rén ‘no one’ are used as answers to negative questions in negative question-answer pairs, the default interpretation of fragments is a DN reading. Consider the example below:

(19) Q. Shéi méiyǒu zài sùshè?

who not.have at dormitory

‘Who is not in the dormitory?’

A. Méiyǒurén.

not.have.people

‘No one.’

=A1. Méiyǒurén (méiyǒu zài sùshè.)

not.have.people not.have at dormitory

‘No one is not in the dormitory.’

The default interpretation of the fragment answer in (19A) to the question in (19Q) is that it conveys a positive reading, because the fragment argumental negative expression of the fragment answer has to combine with the negative operator of the elliptical negative question. In this situation, the two negative expressions (i.e. the fragment argumental negative expression méiyǒurén ‘no one’ and the negative sentential marker méiyǒu ‘not’) cancel each other out, resulting in a DN interpretation.

In contrast, in Spanish, when the n-words such as nadie ‘nobody’ are used as answers to negative questions, they are most commonly interpreted as expressing SN meanings (Espinal & Prieto, 2011; Espinal et. al, 2016; Prieto et. al, 2013). This is illustrated in (20).

(20) Q. ¿Quién no llevaba gafas? (Spanish)

who not wore glasses

‘Who wasn’t wearing glasses?’

A. Nadie.

nobody

‘Nobody.’ = (Nobody was wearing glasses.)

(example from Espinal and Tubau 2016:44, ex. (6))

Based on the assumption that n-words in Romance languages have underspecified formal features (Espinal, 2000; Martins, 2000; Rooryck, 1994, van der Wouden & Zwarts, 1993), the negative meaning of fragment n-words in (20A) is postulated to yield SN interpretations because it stands in a sort of syntactic agreement or semantic dependency with the negative marker no in the question. That is, fragment n-words have been argued not to encode a negative logical operator in NC languages. The fragment word in (20A) is expected to be interpreted as conveying SN (Espinal, 2000).

However, interestingly, an increasing number of studies in the literature on both Romance and Germanic languages show that meaning shifts are possible from expected SN readings to unexpected DN interpretations, and vice versa (Prieto & Espinal, 2020; de Swart, 2020).

Espinal and Prieto (2011) show the interaction between fragment n-words and a contradictory contour (a rising pitch accent associated with the stressed syllable followed by a low-rising boundary tone: L+H* L!H% in Cat_ToBI and Sp_ToBI). Prieto et al. (2013) and Espinal et al. (2016) show for both, Catalan and Spanish, the role of congruent and incongruent gestures, and confirm the role of the contradictory intonation contour not only for fragment n-words but also for preverbal n-words, with or without the co-presence of a sentential negative marker. Depréz and Yeaton (2018) show similar results for French, according to which the combination of a specific syntax and a special prosody can yield a DN reading in this language as well.

Blanchette (2017) shows that, under a specific syntactic structure, a SN reading is preferred by Standard English native speakers. Blanchette and Nadeu (2018), following Espinal and Prieto (2011), confirm experimentally the hypothesis that Standard English is a language that assigns to both SN and DN interpretations negative indefinite fragment answers that are replies to negative questions, being DN readings prosodically marked with a higher fundamental frequency (f0) than their SN negation counterparts.

Experimental studies in Mandarin Chinese vs. Spanish Experimental study by <xref ref-type="bibr" rid="redalyc_672373384009_ref25">Prieto et al. 2013</xref>

Prieto et al. (2013) investigate the extent to which prosodic and gestural patterns affect the interpretation of DN in two Negative Concord languages, Catalan and Spanish. However, in this study I will only focus on Spanish.

Some audiovisual recordings were first recorded by four Spanish native speakers. Participants were asked to answer negative questions using a target word nadie ‘nobody,’ after they were provided with a context and a question. They were asked to express either a negative or a positive meaning using nadie ‘nobody’. Some representative audiovisual recordings were selected for the next perception experiment.

A total of 30 native Spanish speakers were recruited to participate in the perception experiment. The perception experiment consists of three modalities: auditory-only (AO), visual-only (VO), and audiovisual (AV). Participants were provided with a context and a question, and then were asked to listen to or watch a target answer nadie ‘nobody’ to the question. The task of participants is to indicate whether the answer elicits a single negation or a double negation meaning.

The results of this experimental study show that in the AO modality, participants preferred a DN interpretation when the negative word was produced with a contradictory intonational contour; in the VO modality, participants preferred a DN meaning when the negative word was produced with specific denial gestures; in the AV modality, DN interpretations were preferred when the negative word was produced with prosodic and gestural patterns. In conclusion, this study points out that DN meaning of the negative word nadie ‘nobody’ is possible in a so-called NC language in the condition of specific prosodic and gestural patterns.

Experimental study by <xref ref-type="bibr" rid="redalyc_672373384009_ref20">Li 2019</xref>

In 2019, I investigated the interpretation of fragment argumental negative expressions as answers to negative questions in MC.

An audio production experiment was first conducted to investigate whether native MC speakers can produce fragment argumental negative expressions that convey either a SN meaning or DN meaning, and which of these prosodic correlates are for SN and DN respectively.

A total of 30 participants were recruited. After reading a context and a question, each participant was asked to answer the question spontaneously and then answer it using fragments (没(有)人méi(yǒu)rén ‘no one’ or 没(有)什么méi(yǒu)shénme ‘nothing’).

The results show that MC speakers prefer to reply to negative wh- questions in SN negations by means of negative sentences with a negative marker (such as méi(yǒu)rén dào le ‘No one has arrived.’) rather than with a fragment argumental negative expression (such as méi(yǒu)rén ‘no one’). When used as fragment answers to negative wh-questions, the two negative expressions were produced with different prosodic correlates in DN contexts than in SN contexts, éiyǒu segments[3] being more emphatic when associated with a DN meaning than with a SN meaning. Specifically, éiyǒu segments in the DN contexts were found to display a shorter duration, a higher mean pitch, wider pitch variation, and a higher maximum pitch than in the SN contexts.

The second production experiment aimed to investigate whether MC speakers use gestural strategies when expressing a SN or a DN reading. A total of four participants were recruited. They read a context, listen to the question, and then answer the question using fragments. However, results revealed that gesture strategies in this experiment were not significant.

The third audio perception experiment was conducted to investigate whether native MC speakers perceive certain prosodic/acoustic correlates of fragment argumental negative expressions as more appropriate to certain contexts. A total of 101 participants were recruited. After reading a context and a question, they listened to two recordings (SN or DN). Then they were asked to choose the one they felt to be more appropriate and choose the more salient interpretation of their audio choice. The result shows that native speakers of MC perceive differences between audio recordings of fragment negative expressions, depending on their prosodic/acoustic correlates.

Mandarin Chinese vs. Spanish

The analysis of this section is based on a previous experimental study by Prieto et al. (2013), and the experimental study on Mandarin Chinese conducted by Li (2019). The aim of the current research is, first, to compare the prosodic and gesture strategies used by native speakers in the expression of fragments to negative questions in Mandarin Chinese and Spanish. Second, to question the parametric division between DN and NC languages.

Results in Spanish show that, when isolated n-words are uttered with a contradiction intonation (L+H*L!H%), DN interpretation is preferred. The result in MC shows that the fragment answers are more emphatic (higher mean pitch, wider pitch variation, a higher maximum pitch and larger rising pitch excursion) when expressing a DN meaning. The results from these two experimental studies show clear evidence that prosody plays an important role in the interpretations of isolated n-words/negative fragment answers in both NC and DN languages.

Results in Spanish shows that gesture strategies (shrugging, head nod and head shake) also have an important role in the interpretation of DN readings of n-words. However, no significant gesture patterns are found in MC.

The most important common finding from these two studies is that a DN interpretation of n-words of a negative question is possible in a NC language, and that a SN interpretation of a negative fragment answer to a negative question is possible in a DN language. The results support the hypothesis that the parametric division between DN and NC languages is not clear-cut.

Conclusions

In this study, the basic expressions of negation in MC and Spanish were introduced. In the traditional literature, MC has been categorized as a DN language and Spanish has been categorized as a NC language. We have compared the interpretation of multiple negative expressions and fragment answers to negative questions in these two languages.

The default interpretation of multiple negative expressions within a single sentence is DN and SN in MC and Spanish respectively. In MC, when multiple negative expressions co-occur in one sentence, they generally cancel each other out to yield a DN reading (--p ⟹ p). In contrast, in Spanish, when multiple negative expressions are combined in a sentence, the sentence typically leads to a single negation reading (¬p), in spite of the fact that, within the limit of a sentence, the negative marker combines with an indefinite expression that can also convey a negative meaning.

The default interpretation of a fragment answer to a negative question in MC is DN. For instance, to answer a negative wh- question such as shéi méiyǒu qù jiàoshi? ‘who didn’t go swimming’, the default interpretation of a fragment answer such as 没有人méi(yǒu)rén ‘no one’ is DN, in which the fragment answer is interpreted to mean ‘no one didn’t go swimming’, that is, ‘everyone went swimming’. The default meaning of a fragment answer to a negative question in Spanish is SN. For example,

to answer a negative wh- question such as ¿quién no ha comido queso ‘who didn’t eat cheese?’, the default interpretation of a fragment answer such as nadie ‘nobody’ is SN, in which the fragment answer is interpreted to mean ‘nobody ate cheese.’

However, the distinction between DN and NC is not macroparametric; this is mostly due to different readings attributed to sentences that show different syntactic and prosodic patterns. The two experimental studies show that a DN and a NC interpretation is possible in Spanish and MC respectively. At the same time, they show that both verbal and non-verbal strategies also have an important role in the interpretation of negation in both MC and Spanish. A DN interpretation in Spanish is preferred when fragment n-words are associated with a contradiction intonation; fragment answers in MC expressing a DN meaning are more emphatic. Gestures (shrugging, head nod and head shake) are found to be associated with DN reading in Spanish. The interaction between negation and gestures in MC remains to be studied in the future, considering different circumstances and conditions.

To sum up, the two experimental studies both indicate that unexpected DN or SN readings are possible under certain conditions in Spanish and MC respectively. Consequently, the division between DN and NC languages is called into question. In addition, DN is prosodically marked in both MC and Spanish. Further studies on the interpretation of negation across languages and the interactions between verbal and non-verbal cues are expected in the future.

Acknowledgments

I thank the anonymous reviewer for his/her useful comments that helped me improve this article. This research has been supported by grants from the Youth Fund Project of Research Planning Foundation on Humanities and Social Sciences of the Ministry of Education, China (22YJC740032), the Sichuan Federation of Social Science Associations (SC22C066) and the University of Electronic Science and Technology of China (Y030212059003004).

<bold>REFERENCES</bold> Biberauer, Theresa and Zeijlstra Hedde. (2012). Negative Concord in Afrikaans: Filling a typological gap. Journal of Semantics, 29, 345-271. Biberauer Theresa Zeijlstra Hedde Negative Concord in Afrikaans: Filling a typological gap Journal of Semantics 2012 Blanchette, Frances. (2017). Microsyntactic variation in English Negative Concord. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 2, 1-32. Blanchette Frances Microsyntactic variation in English Negative Concord Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 2017 1 32 Blanchette, Frances and Nadeu, Marianna. (2018). Prosody and the meanings of English negative indefinites. Journal of Pragmatics, 129, 123-139. Blanchette Frances Nadeu Marianna Prosody and the meanings of English negative indefinites Journal of Pragmatics 2018 129 123 139 Bosque, Ignacio. (1980). Sobre la negación. Cátedra. Bosque Ignacio Cátedra 1980 Cheng, Lisa Lai-Shen and Li, Yafei. (1991). Double negation in Chinese and multi projections. In Paper presented at the North America Conference on Chinese Linguistics. Cornell University Press. Cheng Lisa Lai-Shen Li Yafei Double negation in Chinese and multi projections. Paper presented at the North America Conference on Chinese Linguistics 1991 de Swart, Henriëtte, (2020). Double negation readings. In Viviane Déprez and M.Teresa Espinal (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Negation. Oxford University Press. de Swart Henriëtte Oxford University Press 2020 Double negation readings Déprez, Viviane and Yeaton, Jeremy. 2018. French Negative Concord and Discord: An experimental investigation of contextual and prosodic disambiguation. In Repetti Lori and Ordóñez Francisco (Ed.), Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory 14 (pp. 33-51). John Benjamins. Déprez Viviane Yeaton Jeremy Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory 2018 33 51 French Negative Concord and Discord: An experimental investigation of contextual and prosodic disambiguation Ding, Shengshu et al. (1999). Xiandai Hanyu Yufa Jianghua (Lectures in Modern Chinese Grammar). Commercial Press. Ding Shengshu Commercial Press 1999 Xiandai Hanyu Yufa Jianghua (Lectures in Modern Chinese Grammar) Ernst, Thomas. (1995). Negation in Mandarin Chinese. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 13, 665-707. Ernst Thomas Negation in Mandarin Chinese Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 1995 13 665 707 Espinal, M.Teresa. (2000). On the semantic status of n-words in Catalan and Spanish. Lingua, 110, 557-580. Espinal M.Teresa On the semantic status of n-words in Catalan and Spanish Lingua 2000 110 557 580 Espinal, M.Teresa and Prieto, Pilar. (2011). Intonational encoding of Double Negation in Catalan. Journal of Pragmatics, 43, 2392-2410. Espinal M.Teresa Prieto Pilar Intonational encoding of Double Negation in Catalan Journal of Pragmatics 2011 43 2392 2410 Espinal, M.Teresa, Tubau, Susagna, Borrás-Comes, Joan and Prieto, Pilar. (2016). Double negation in Catalan and Spanish: Interaction between syntax and prosody. In Pierre Larrivée and Chungmin Lee (Ed.), Negation and Polarity: Experimental Perspectives (pp. 145-176). Springer. Espinal M.Teresa Negation and Polarity: Experimental Perspectives 2016 Double negation in Catalan and Spanish: Interaction between syntax and prosody Giannakidou, Anastasia. (1998). Polarity Sensitivity as (Non)Veridical Dependency. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Giannakidou Anastasia John Benjamins 1998 Amsterdam Polarity Sensitivity as (Non)Veridical Dependency Giannakidou, Anastasia. (2006). N-words and Negative Concord. In Martin Everaert, Rob Goedemans and Henk van Riemsdijk (Ed.), The Syntax Companian (volume 3) (pp. 327-391). Blackwell. Giannakidou Anastasia The Syntax Companian (volume 3) 2006 327 391 Blackwell N-words and Negative Concord Horn, Laurence R. (1989). A Natural History of Negation. University of Chicago Press. Horn Laurence R University of Chicago Press 1989 A Natural History of Negation Huang, Borong and Liao, Xudong. (2008). Xiandai Hanyu (Modern Chinese). Higher Education Press. Huang Borong Liao Xudong Higher Education Press 2008 Xiandai Hanyu (Modern Chinese) Huddlestone, Kate. (2010). Negative indefinites in Afrikaans. Ph.D. dissertation, Utrecht University. Huddlestone Kate Utrecht University 2010 Negative indefinites in Afrikaans Laka, Itziar. (1990). Negation in syntax: on the nature of functional categories and projection. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Laka Itziar Massachusetts Institute of Technology 1990 Negation in syntax: on the nature of functional categories and projection Li, Charles. N. and Thompson, Sandra. A. (1981). Mandarin Chinese: A Functional Reference Grammar. University of California Press. Li Charles. N Thompson Sandra. A University of California Press 1981 Mandarin Chinese: A Functional Reference Grammar Li, Feifei. (2019). On the interpretation of negation in Mandarin Chinese. Ph.D. dissertation. Li Feifei Ph.D. dissertation 2019 On the interpretation of negation in Mandarin Chinese Li, Mei. (2007). Negation in Chinese. Shanghai Foreign Language Education Press. Li Mei Shanghai Foreign Language Education Press 2007 Negation in Chinese Lü, Shuxiang. (1985). Yiwen, fouding, kending (Question, negation and affirmation). Zhongguo Yuwen, 4, 241-250. Shuxiang Yiwen, fouding, kending (Question, negation and affirmation) Zhongguo Yuwen 1985 4 241 250 Lü, Shuxiang. (1999). Xiandai hanyu babai ci (Eight Hundred Words of Modern Chinese). Commercial Press. Shuxiang Commercial Press 1999 Xiandai hanyu babai ci (Eight Hundred Words of Modern Chinese) Martins, Ana Maria. (2000). Polarity items in Romance: Underspecification and lexical change. In Susan Pintzuk, Tsoulas George and Anthony Warner (Ed.), Diachronic Syntax: Models and Mechanisms (pp.191-219). Oxford University Press. Martins Ana Maria Oxford University Press 2000 Polarity items in Romance: Underspecification and lexical change Prieto, Pilar, Borràs-Comes, Joan, Tubau, Susagna and Espinal, M.Teresa. (2013). Prosody and gesture constrain the interpretation of double negation. Lingua, 131, 136-150. Prieto Pilar Prosody and gesture constrain the interpretation of double negation Lingua 2013 131 136 150 Prieto, Pilar and Espinal, M.Teresa. (2020). Negation and prosody. In Viviane Déprez and M.Teresa Espinal (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Negation. Oxford University Press. Prieto Pilar Espinal M.Teresa Oxford University Press 2020 Negation and prosody Puskás, Genoveva. (2012). Licensing double negation in NC and non-NC languages. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 30, 611-649. Puskás Genoveva Licensing double negation in NC and non-NC languages Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 2012 30 611 649 Rooryck, Johan. (1994). On two types of underspecification: Towards a feature theory shared by syntax and phonology. Probus, 6, 207-233. Rooryck Johan On two types of underspecification: Towards a feature theory shared by syntax and phonology Probus 1994 6 207 233 Sánchez López, Cristina. (1999). La negación. In Ignacio Bosque and Violeta Demonte (Ed.), Gramática descriptiva de la lengua española (pp. 2561-2634). Espasa-Calpe. Sánchez López Cristina Espasa-Calpe 1999 La negación Thornton, Rosalind, Notley, Anna, Moscati, Vincenzo and Crain, Stephen. (2016). Two negations for the price of one. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 45, 1-30. Thornton Rosalind Two negations for the price of one Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 2016 45 1 30 Van der Wouden, Ton. (1994). Negative contexts. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Groningen. Van der Wouden Ton University of Groningen 1994 Negative contexts Van der Wouden, Ton and Zwarts, Frans. (1993). A semantic analysis of negative concord. In Utpal Lahiri and Adam Zachary Wyner (Ed.), Proceedings from Semantics and Linguistic Theory III (pp.202-219). Cornell University Press. Van der Wouden Ton Zwarts Frans Cornell University Press 1993 A semantic analysis of negative concord Xiao, Richard and McEnery, Tony. (2008). Negation in Mandarin Chinese: A Corpus-based Study.Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 36(2), 274-330. Xiao Richard McEnery Tony Negation in Mandarin Chinese: A Corpus-based Study Journal of Chinese Linguistics 36 2 274 330 Yang, Huiling. (2011). Is Chinese a Negative Concord Language? In Jing-Schmidt Zhuo (Ed.), proceedings of the 23rd North American Conference on Chinese Linguistics (NACCL-23) (pp. 208-223). University of Oregon Press. Yang Huiling University of Oregon Press 2011 Is Chinese a Negative Concord Language? Zeijlstra, Hedde. (2004). Sentential Negation and Negative Concord. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Amsterdam Press. Zeijlstra Hedde University of Amsterdam Press 2004 Sentential Negation and Negative Concord Zhuang, Huibin and Liu, Zhenqian. (2011). Negative marker bu in Chinese: Its nature and features. International Journal of Asian Language Processing, 21, 107-160. Zhuang Huibin Liu Zhenqian Negative marker bu in Chinese: Its nature and features International Journal of Asian Language Processing 21 107 160 Zhang, Huanxiang. (2012). On the categories of double negation in Chinese and English. Ph.D. dissertation, Capital Normal University. Zhang Huanxiang Capital Normal University 2012 On the categories of double negation in Chinese and English Zhou, Peng, Crain, Stephen and Thornton, Rosalind. 2014. Children’s knowledge of double negative structures in Mandarin Chinese. Journal East Asian Linguist, 23, 333-359. Zhou Peng Crain Stephen Thornton Rosalind Children’s knowledge of double negative structures in Mandarin Chinese Journal East Asian Linguist 2014 23 333 359 Zhuang, Huibin. (2015). Xiandai hanyu fouding de jufa yanjiu (Negation in Modern Chinese: A syntactic Perspective). Science Press. Zhuang Huibin Science Press 2015 Xiandai hanyu fouding de jufa yanjiu (Negation in Modern Chinese: A syntactic Perspective) Notes

An anonymous reviewer of this article also mentioned another difference between 不 and 没(有)méi(yǒu). 不 is a stative verb, activity verb, and modal verb. 没(有)méi(yǒu) for anything marked with aspect ‘在zai’ is for progressive, ‘了-le’ for perfective, ‘过-guo’ for experiential, and ‘着zhe’ for durative (Li and Thompson 1989).

An anonymous reviewer of this article indicated that Spanish no doesn't distinguish what is being negated 'verb' or 'noun' – it is more like a general negator.

Here éiyǒu segments are from méiyǒurén and méiyǒushénme. The region of occurrence of the first vocalic sequence of the two negative words méiyǒurén and méiyǒushénme (i.e., éiyǒu) was delimited for specific reasons.

Refbacks

  • There are currently no refbacks.



Estadísticas
Visitas al Abstract:317



{PSOCIAL} Journal of Research in Social Psychology. Faculty of Social Sciences | University of Buenos Aires (UBA)

ISSN 2422-619X. Semiannual publication (January-June and July-December).
 
Design: Mae Bermudez
 

 Jorunal Indexed and listed in:
  • ERIH PLUS (European Reference Index for the Humanities and Social Sciences) [registry]
  • Latindex Catálogo 2.0 (Regional Cooperative Online Information System for Scholarly Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portuga) [registry]
  • DOAJ (Directory Open Access Journals) [registry]
  • MIAR (Information Matrix for the Analysis of Journals) [registry]
  • PSICODOC (Online Bibliographic Database Madrid Official College of Psychologists) [registry
  • RDIUBA (Institutional Digital Repository) [registry]
  • REDIB (Iberoamerican Network for Innovation and Scientific Knowledge) [registry]
  • Open AIRE (Open Access Infraestructure for Research in Europe) [registry
  • Red LatinRev / FLACSO library (Latin American Faculty of Social Sciences) [registry]
  • BINPAR (National Bibliography of Registered Periodicals) [registry]
  • LATINOAMERICANA (Association of Academic Journals of Humanities and Social Sciences) [registry]
  • CLASE (Latin-American Citations in Social Sciences and Humanities) [registry]
  • Sherpa Romeo [registry]
  • Basic Nucleus of Argentine Scientific Journals [registry]
  • Mirab@l [registry]
  • Cabells' Journalytics [registry]
  • CIRC (Integrated Classification of Scientific Journals) [registry]
  • AmeliCA [registry]
  • LILACS (Latin American and Caribbean Health Sciences Literature [registry]
  • EBSCO (Elton Bryson Stephens Company Information Services) [registry]
  • Malena [registry
  • Sara Network [registry]
  • SciELO (Scientific Electronic Library Online) [registry]
  • Redalyc (Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America and the Caribbean, Spain and. Portuga) [registry]
 

This journal is licenced under Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-SA 4.0)